If you don't remember your password, you can reset it by entering your email address and clicking the Reset Password button. You will then receive an email that contains a secure link for resetting your password
If the address matches a valid account an email will be sent to __email__ with instructions for resetting your password
Address for correspondence: Lindsay M. Jaacks, PhD, Department of Global Health and Population, Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health, Harvard University, Bldg 1, Rm 1211, 665 Huntington Ave, Boston, MA 02115; Phone: (617) 432-2505; Fax: (617) 432-6133;
To understand factors that influence the process of food decision-making among urban Indian women in the context of the nutrition transition.
Design
Mixed methods.
Methods
Semistructured interviews and sorting of 12 a priori hypothesized drivers into categories of always, sometimes, and never influencing food choice.
Setting
Delhi, India.
Participants
Thirty-eight women aged 20–35 years selected from the roster of a prospective cohort study.
Phenomenon of Interest
Drivers of food choice.
Analysis
Interviews were transcribed verbatim and translated, then analyzed using an iterative, constant comparative process. Differences in pile sort rankings across tertiles of body mass index and wealth index were tested using Fisher exact tests.
Result
Four primary themes emerged: (1) family influence; (2) cultural perceptions, with 3 subthemes of beliefs relating to (a) outside food and less healthful food, (b) seasonality, and (c) hometown food; (3) convenience, with 3 subthemes of (a) decisions regarding procurement of food, (b) not having time to cook, resulting in eating out or purchasing premade foods, and (c) eating whatever is available at home or is left over from previous meals; and (4) habit, with 2 subthemes of (a) subconscious decisions and (b) food roots. Responses from the pile sort activity revealed perceptions that food safety and health most influence food choice whereas marketing and advertisements least influence it.
Conclusions and Implications
Young adult women in Delhi seem to rely on preferences of their families, habits and perceptions established in childhood, convenience, and food safety and health when making choices about food. These aspects of decision-making should be targeted in future interventions aimed at improving dietary intake in this population.
: consumption of energy-dense, processed foods such as fried and salted snacks and prepared sweets is increasing. Moreover, although carbohydrates still account for a large proportion of total energy intake (73% in rural areas and 68% in urban areas
), coarse grains (whole-wheat flour, brown rice, millet, barley, and sorghum) are being replaced with refined grains (white flour and polished white rice).
Traditional diets in India vary considerably across regions, with whole wheat–based breads (eg, chapatis or roti) contributing a majority of calories in the north and rice and lentils (including dosa [rice-lentil crepe] and idli [steamed rice-lentil cake]) being the major contributors in the south. Traditionally, dairy products, with the exception of yogurt, are more common in the north (eg, milk, ghee [clarified butter], and paneer [cheese]) and coconut is more common in the south. A meal typically consists of a staple grain with seasonal vegetables (depending on the recipe, the cooking method could be shallow or deep frying) and sometimes a pulse/lentil-based dish (pressure-cooked).
The nutrition transition in India has been accompanied by an epidemiological transition, particularly in urban areas. A recent analysis by the India State-level Disease Burden Initiative found that in 2016, noncommunicable diseases (NCDs) such as cardiovascular disease, diabetes, and cancer accounted for 61.8% of deaths compared with undernutrition and communicable, maternal, and neonatal diseases, which together accounted for just 27.5% of deaths.
They also found that in 1990, Delhi, which is located in the northern part of India, was among the first areas in India to experience the epidemiological transition; it had more deaths attributable to NCDs than undernutrition and communicable, maternal, and neonatal diseases at that time.
Delhi, which is the nation's capital and second largest city (the population according to the 2011 Census was approximately 11 million), is also uniquely diverse in its food culture, although heavily influenced by traditional northern Indian cuisines. Given this evidence of a substantial and increasing burden of NCDs in India, and Delhi specifically, interventions targeting key risk factors for NCDs, such as dietary intake, are urgently needed.
To develop such interventions, a greater understanding of underlying drivers of dietary behaviors in India is needed. As outlined by the Food Choice Process Model, decisions relating to food choice are frequent, complex, and multifaceted, and vary over both personal and historical time.
Research conducted in high-income countries found that cultural ideals are among the most important drivers of food choice, as are personal factors such as taste preference and identities.
Only 1 previous study explored drivers of food choice in the context of India. That study, which was conducted in rural Kerala, the southernmost state of India, found that food choices within the household were strongly influenced by the preferences of husbands and children, as well as cost.
the 2 objectives of this study were (1) to determine the drivers of food choice among young adult women living in Delhi and (2) to interpret these drivers in the context of the nutrition and epidemiological transitions.
Methods
Participants and Recruitment
The researchers used a combination of semistructured and structured qualitative methods to identify drivers of food choice among women aged 20–35 years living in Delhi. A total of 38 women were purposively selected from the roster of a prospective cohort study called the Centre for Cardiometabolic Risk Reduction in South Asia (CARRS) Surveillance Study.
The CARRS study used multistage probability sampling to select a representative sample of Delhi at baseline, in 2014, and has continued to follow up with participants annually to collect data on incident disease. To be eligible for inclusion in the CARRS study, individuals had to be living in Delhi for ≥5 years with no plans to move from Delhi in the next 5 years.
For the purposes of this mixed-methods study, 2 categories of body mass index (BMI) and tertiles of household wealth index were calculated using the CARRS dataset for women age 20–35 years. Body mass index was derived as weight (in kilograms) divided by height squared (in meters) using measured values of weight and height obtained by trained field staff. Wealth index was derived using principal component analysis based on household amenities (separate cooking room and toilet facilities) and assets (television, refrigerator, washing machine, microwave, mixer-grinder, mobile phone, DVD player, computer, car, motorized bike, and bicycle).
Women were enrolled from each category of BMI-by-wealth index (Table 1).
Table 1Distribution for Sample Selection of Women Aged 20–35 Y (n = 38)
Body Mass Index, kg/m2
Wealth Index (Tertile)
1
2
3
18.5–24.9
6
6
6
≥25
6
6
8
Note: Data are shown as n. Wealth index was derived using principal component analysis based on household amenities (separate cooking room and toilet facilities) and assets (television, refrigerator, washing machine, microwave, mixer-grinder, mobile phone, DVD player, computer, car, motorized bike, and bicycle).
Sociodemographic characteristics including age, educational attainment, employment status, household size, monthly household income, and religion were also obtained from the CARRS dataset. The CARRS survey questions were derived from questionnaires used in the Chennai Urban Population Study, the Chennai Urban Rural Epidemiological Study, and the Sentinel Surveillance Study,
and all surveys were administered in person by trained field staff.
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and Harvard University, and the Centre for Chronic Disease Control Ethics Committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were not compensated for their participation.
Semistructured Interviews
The interview guide (Supplementary Data) was adapted from a previous study on drivers of food choice among women and men residing in Kerala, India,
The initial interview script was pilot-tested with 3 young adult women. The initial interview guide asked only about health perceptions of the foods mentioned by participants in the dietary recall. After the pilot interviews, the guide was revised to probe more generally about perceptions of healthy and unhealthy foods. Questions about whom the participant sought for dietary advice and how the participant's eating habits had changed since youth were also added to the guide after pilot interviews. Finally, some questions were revised to be more open-ended, and more detailed probes were added as sub-questions under each main question to ensure the interviews resulted in rich data.
Analysis of these pilot interviews identified key issues and concepts that were used to revise the interview guide before use in the field. Each field interview was conducted in the home of the participant and lasted 20–40 minutes. With the permission of the participant, all interviews were audio-recorded. Interviews were conducted between September and November, 2016 by nutritionists trained in qualitative methods (DK and VG) in the participant's native language (Hindi), with the exception of 1 interview conducted by CB in English. Audio recordings were transcribed verbatim and translated into English by a hired, trained translator and English transcripts were verified by DK and VG.
Each interview began with a standard dietary recall (except that the amount of intake was not obtained) to elicit information about the types of foods and beverages consumed by participants in the previous 24 hours. The core of each interview consisted of open-ended questions about why the participant decided to consume each food or beverage mentioned during the recall and whether the food or beverage was typical of the participant's usual diet. After the pilot tests, more specific prompts about cost, physical environment, health, family members, and media were added to the guide. Further questions attempted to discern whether the participant's current diet was similar to or different from her diet during childhood, and whether her current diet was similar to or different from her parents' diets when they were her age. Asking these specific questions deepened the understanding of her food choices in the context of the nutrition and epidemiological transitions.
Pile Sorts
At the end of the interview, participants were asked to complete a pile sort activity in which they classified 12 a priori hypothesized drivers of food choice based on the previously mentioned study in Kerala, India,
into categories of always influences food choice, sometimes influences food choice, and never influences food choice. The 12 drivers in the pile sort activity were as follows: hunger/appetite, taste, cost of food/income, time available, mood, knowledge about food, cooking skills, health, food safety, packaging/labeling, marketing/advertisements, and attitudes/beliefs about certain foods (including cultural beliefs/traditions). The initial pile sorting activity asked participants to rank the 12 drivers in terms of how much influence each driver had on their food choice relative to the other drivers. However, after pilot-testing, it became clear that this was too challenging for participants, so the pile sorting activity was revised to ask participants to sort the 12 drivers into the 3 categories of always, sometimes, or never.
Data Analysis
The researchers conducted analysis of the qualitative data using Dedoose (version 7.0.23, SocioCultural Research Consultants, LLC, Los Angeles, CA, 2016). The research team members read and reviewed transcripts individually, followed by a group meeting to discuss emergent codes, categories, concepts, and themes. The first review and meeting occurred after the transcription of the 3 pilot interviews to ensure the quality of the data and allow for any necessary adjustments to the interview guide. An initial codebook was created with codes borrowed from a study on food choices by Furst et al.
After this first meeting and the development of an initial codebook by CB, LMJ, HW, VG, and DK, CB individually coded subsequent transcripts, followed by additional meetings and adjustment of the interview guide and codebook as needed. Inductive codes were added to the initial codebook as additional concepts emerged from the interview transcripts. To improve reproducibility, all codes were clearly defined and mutually exclusive. Following Campbell et al,
2 types of reliability were assessed: stability and reproducibility (eg, intercoder reliability and agreement). Stability, or whether a coder's use of codes changes over time,
was addressed by LMJ, who reviewed all coded transcripts for consistency. Reproducibility was addressed by VG and DK, who independently coded 20% of transcripts. Of the 244 excerpts in CB's coded transcripts, 38 had a discrepant code compared with VG and DK's coded transcripts (85% agreement). After discussions by CB, LMJ, VG, and DK, all discrepancies were reconciled. Coding reports were exported from Dedoose and analyzed using a constant comparative method
in: Pequegnat W. Stover E. Boyce C.A. How to Write a Successful Research Grant Application: A Guide for Social and Behavioral Scientists. 2nd ed. Springer,
New York, NY2011: 153-172
by CB and LMJ. Original categories arising from the interviews were applied to the cumulative data and revised multiple times until there was group consensus about their fit.
Analysis of the pile sort activity was conducted using SAS (version 9.4, SAS Institute, Cary, NC, 2013). Differences in pile sort rankings (always, sometimes, or never) across tertiles of BMI and wealth index were tested using Fisher exact tests. P < .05 was considered statistically significant.
Results
The mean age of participants was 29.5 years and most were homemakers (Table 2). Four primary themes emerged: (1) family influence, (2) cultural perceptions, (3) convenience, and (4) habits (Figure 1). These are each discussed in detail in subsequent text, followed by results of the structured technique (pile sorts).
Table 2Demographic Characteristics of Study Participants (n = 38)
Characteristics
% (n)
Age, y (mean [SD])
29.5 (4.3)
Highest education level attained
Illiterate
3 (1)
Literate, no formal education
5 (2)
Primary school
3 (1)
High school
21 (8)
Secondary school
26 (10)
University graduate
32 (12)
Professional degree/postgraduate
11 (4)
Employment status
Housewife
84 (32)
Employed
13 (5)
Student
3 (1)
Household size (mean [SD])
Adults (age ≥ 18 y)
2.8 (1.1)
Children (age < 18 y)
1.8 (1.0)
Estimated total household income/mo (INR)
3,000–10,000
24 (9)
10,001–20,000
40 (15)
20,001–30,000
18 (7)
30,001–40,000
3 (1)
40,001–50,000
8 (3)
>50,000
5 (2)
Do not know
3 (1)
Religion
Hindu
95 (36)
Muslim
3 (1)
Christian
3 (1)
INR indicates Indian rupee.
Note: Demographic data were collected as part of the Cardiometabolic Risk Reduction in South Asia Surveillance Study.
For nearly all women interviewed, husbands, children, and in-laws were a primary driver of food choice. Mothers-in-law influenced decisions about food procurement (Table 3). Children and husbands were especially influential regarding decisions related to eating away from home. This influence often led to unhealthy food decisions:Sometimes [I have a snack] when children bring something like samosa [deep-fried refined wheat Indian snack stuffed with potato and peas] and like chow mein [noodles] (age 27, 23.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1).
Table 3Direct Comparison of Food Choice Process Model and Constructs From Conceptual Framework of Drivers of Food Choice Among Young Adult Women in Delhi, India, as Well as Representative Quotations From Study Participants
Earlier, my mother-in-law or my mother used to prepare gulab jamun [Indian sweet, a deep-fried cottage cheese ball that is boiled in sugar syrup], sweet mathri [a deep-fried refined wheat snack], and salty mathri, sweet and savory snacks. But I don't have time, and also, all the items are easily available in the market. People nowadays are working. They don't have time to cook a variety of food, so they prefer to cook less and purchase that from markets.
Age 26, BMI 22.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
Social factors
Family
My mother-in-law, because she gets the entire stuff for home, like spices and all. My mother-in-law gets all that.
Age 30, BMI 24.9 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
Now, my husband likes food from outside, so I also started eating outside food.
Age 28, BMI 24.9 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
Trajectories and transitions
Food roots
I [ate] meat and egg more often in Bihar because my family members were not vegetarian, and here my husband is a pure vegetarian, so I eat meat and egg less often here in Delhi.
Age 20, BMI 21.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1
Earlier, I used to eat much less spicy and oily food. Now I eat more spicy and oily food … Earlier, I didn't eat non-veg. Now, I started eating non-veg after marriage.
Age 33, BMI 29.0 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
… We do not eat green vegetables now. My husband does not like to eat.
Age 26, BMI 22.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
I eat rice at night and I want to change it but can't, because my husband wants rice at night daily, and I don't want to cook separate food for myself.
Age 27, BMI 31.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
Yes, there is a change after marriage, as here, we use a lot of oil for cooking.
Age 33, BMI 33.4 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
Ideals
Personal cultural perceptions
Home food is the best. Everything is cooked with clean hands. You see the way it is cooked outside! It seems so unhygienic.
Age 30, BMI 24.9 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
All the junk food like Maggi [prepackaged noodles], burger, and pizza is unhealthy.
Age 21, BMI 22.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
I like chow mein, so sometimes I have this, but I don't like momos, pizza, and other junk food.
Age 25, BMI 22.9 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1
Lemonade is also good for children, not in winter, but it is good in summer.
Age 27, BMI 23.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1
I like tea but not coffee and cold drinks. … I cannot have anything cold in winter.
Age 30, BMI 24.9 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
In UP [Uttar Pradesh], things were a lot purer.… The oil is not pure here, the vegetable quality is poor. Even the spices are not sold whole but grinded only.
Age 34, BMI 28.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
Resources
Convenience
I don't think about cost ever while buying. I get all the things under one roof, so I buy from there.
Age 33, BMI 27.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
We normally get [grocery items] from the local vendor shop nearby our house.
Age 21, BMI 22.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2
Mainly I cook variety of things on holidays, like pulses, vegetables, raita, and salad. But on working days it is not possible for me to cook all these things together.
Age 34, BMI 31.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
When he [her son] leaves for school, only then I have time to have my breakfast.
Age 27, BMI 31.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3
Food strategies
Habits
I prepare food in the same way daily. Chapatis and vegetables are fixed for lunch and breakfast.
Age 20, BMI 21.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1
… We are used to having such kinds of foods. We have parathas and rotis and subzis in my diet now, so we are used to it. … These chapatis and subzis, this is the routine of my life, from childhood. So, I'm used to it.
It was evident that women prioritized the preferences of their husband and children over their own preferences; for example, “I try to serve everything for my family, whatever they like to have. I don't get time for myself” (age 33, BMI 29.0 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3), and “Time is always available [to cook] for children” (age 29, BMI 22.6 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2).
Many women identified their marriage, which in some cases involved moving to Delhi from their hometown, as a major life transition related to food choice. There were several instances in which the husband's food patterns took precedence over the woman's food roots, which was sometimes a barrier to making healthier food choices (Table 3).
Cultural Perceptions
Three key subthemes related to cultural perceptions: beliefs relating to (1) outside food and less healthful food, (2) seasonality, and (3) hometown food.
Beliefs relating to outside food and junk food
Although decisions about eating outside food were complex, there was a common underlying perception that outside food was unclean (Table 3). Many women perceived homemade food to be healthy regardless of nutritional content, simply because it was prepared at home. One participant summarized this idea: “Homemade food is healthy and outside junk is not” (age 34, BMI 28.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3). However, there were exceptions in which women expressed a clear knowledge of nutrition: When explicitly asked about healthy and unhealthy foods, a large number of women recognized that oily and fried foods were unhealthy, as supported by the following quotation: “Less oily and less fried, because if we eat more oily, then it will affect our health and I will become fat, too” (age 28, BMI 24.8 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1).
The term junk food was applied by several women to refer to Western foods such as pizza and burgers, and Indian snacks purchased from street vendors such as samosas and momos (dumplings) (Table 3). Packaged foods such as biscuits (main ingredients were refined wheat, sugar, and butter), which were almost always consumed with tea, were never referred to as junk food. In fact, most women felt that tea must be taken with biscuits: “I am used to taking a cup of tea with 1 or 2 biscuits. I can't have empty tea. Only tea causes acidity, so I always take a biscuit” (age 26, BMI 22.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3).
Beliefs relating to seasonality
Seasonality had an important role in food choices, particularly as related to beverages and fruits. Cultural perceptions relating to seasonality regarding beverages were a strong driver, in some cases even stronger than taste preference (Table 3). With respect to fruits, cost and seasonality interacted in driving food decisions; for example, “We don't have fruits daily, we don't get too much salary, but we try to have seasonal fruits once in week” (age 35, BMI 30.6 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1), and “… seasonal fruits are cost friendly” (age 27, BMI 31.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3).
Beliefs relating to hometown food
There was also a common perception that food from the woman's hometown was better in terms of both quality and taste, as demonstrated by the following quotation:From my village to Delhi, it's a drastic change. Here we have less milk and other items, here everyone is keen to eat outside food. I was healthier when I was in my village as compared to Delhi (age 28, BMI 23.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2)
There were some exceptions to this. For example, 1 woman said,In the village, vegetables are not easily available, so we used to eat chapati with buttermilk and chutney, but here in Delhi I started eating vegetables more often because it's easily available (age 30, BMI 27.4 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1).
Convenience
Excerpts from the theme of convenience could be classified into 3 subthemes: (1) decisions regarding procurement of food, (2) not having time to cook, resulting in eating out or purchasing premade foods, and (3) eating whatever is available at home or is left over from previous meals.
Decisions regarding procurement of food
Convenience was especially important in the context of procuring food (Table 3). For some women, there was an interplay between time and cost, especially for nonperishable foods; for example,I prefer to buy all the dry food items once in month … it's time and money saving. As often, there are offers on food items. So, we purchase in bulk for the whole month (age 26, BMI 22.3 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3)
and “… It [Big Bazaar Outlet] is near to my home. It's affordable when we buy in bulk” (age 28, BMI 24.8 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1). The close proximity of a store also had a large role in where women decided to procure their food, especially with respect to fruits and vegetables. One participant, when asked why she bought vegetables only from the nearby shop, explained, “It saves my time because it is near our home … because it's near and I don't have much time to go far to buy all these” (age 32, BMI 22.6 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2).
Not having time to cook, resulting in eating out or purchasing premade foods
Several women found that eating outside food was convenient when they did not have time to cook, particularly for those in the highest wealth tertile and the few women in this sample who were employed. Three of these women explicitly said that they cooked less variety owing to their work schedules (Table 3). Several women who were homemakers also commented that they did not have time to eat owing to chores and sometimes delayed or skipped meals. The dominance of quotations from women in the highest wealth tertile suggests that a higher wealth index allows families to prioritize convenience rather than devoting time to meal preparation. There were some exceptions, with women still refusing to eat outside food despite time pressures: “We always have the time. Basically, for eating we always have the time” (age 32, BMI 25.7 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3). Not having time to cook was also mentioned in discussions of festival foods among women in the highest wealth tertile (Table 3).
Eating whatever is available at home or is left over from previous meals
This subtheme was particularly expressed among women from the lowest and middle wealth tertiles. When prompted to recall what she ate on the previous day, 1 participant stated, “I ate the same food which I cooked in the morning again at 2 in the afternoon” (age 20, BMI 21.2 kg/m2, wealth tertile 1). A lack of time was often cited as a reason for repeating meals: “If we don't have time, then we eat leftover food from the previous day” (age 28, BMI 23.5 kg/m2, wealth tertile 2). The same woman noted that sometimes the demands of work made it necessary to prioritize convenience over eating a variety of foods: “Whatever I get in front of me, I cook it. We are working class; our main aim is to fill the stomach and go back to work.”
Habit
There were 2 subthemes relating to habit: (1) subconscious decisions and (2) food roots. Most participants did not consciously make food choices; rather, they consumed the foods they ate because that was what they had always done. The theme of habit appeared most often in interviews when women were talking about food preparation, as supported by the following quotation: “We have fixed food preparations, therefore, we don't think much before preparing food” (age 29, BMI 19.0 kg/m2, wealth tertile 3). Many participants mentioned that food habits from their childhood had led to persistent patterns of food choice in adulthood (Table 3).
Pile Sorts
One participant refused to place health in any category, so there were only 37 total responses for that driver. Food safety and health were the most influential drivers in participants' food choice; 82% and 78% of participants, respectively, placed these drivers in the always category (Table 4).
Table 4Results of Pile Sort Activity in Which Study Participants Sorted 12 Drivers into Categories of Always, Sometimes, or Never Influence Food Choice (n = 38)
Driver
Influences Choice of Food
Always
Sometimes
Never
Food safety
82 (31)
16 (6)
3 (1)
Health
78 (29)
11 (4)
11 (4)
Taste
68 (26)
18 (7)
13 (5)
Knowledge
66 (25)
21 (8)
13 (5)
Attitudes
53 (20)
16 (6)
32 (12)
Time
47 (18)
34 (13)
18 (7)
Cost
42 (16)
24 (9)
34 (13)
Cooking skills
42 (16)
40 (15)
18 (7)
Packaging
34 (13)
32 (12)
34 (13)
Hunger
32 (12)
53 (20)
16 (6)
Mood
29 (11)
58 (22)
16 (6)
Marketing
11 (4)
37 (14)
53 (20)
Note: Data represent the percent (number) of participants.
Marketing/advertisements was the least influential driver of food choice; 53% of participants reported that this never influenced their food choice and only 11% said it always drove their food choice. When probed specifically about the driver of marketing during interviews, no participant said that it influenced her. Figure 2 presents results of the pile sort activity stratified by BMI and wealth index tertile.
Figure 2Pile sort activity responses graphed by (A) body mass index (BMI) category and (B) wealth index tertile. Differences in pile sort rankings (always, sometimes, or never) across tertiles of BMI and wealth index were tested using Fisher exact tests. *P < .05.
There was no significant association between BMI category and the degree of influence of any driver on food choice. There was a significant positive relationship between wealth index tertile and the strength of 3 drivers: health, attitudes/beliefs, and hunger (P < .001, P = .003, and P = .05, respectively) on food choice. Those in the highest wealth index tertile were more likely to say that health, attitudes/beliefs, and hunger always drove their food choice.
Discussion
Consistent with the Food Choice Process Model, this study found that food choice decisions are complex and multifaceted, and that life course events are important for establishing an individual's food choice trajectory.
Young adult women in Delhi rely primarily on the preferences of their families, including their husbands, children, and in-laws, when making choices about food. Cultural perceptions and habits established in childhood, convenience, and food safety and health also have a critical role in the food decision-making process. These results are especially interesting considering the context of the study: Delhi, an Indian megacity that has experienced rapid economic growth (the gross domestic product per capita in Delhi increased by nearly 13% from 2014–2015 to 2015–2016
The results indicate that the nutrition transition is well under way in Delhi and highlight important opportunities for interventions to prevent the rising threat of NCDs such as diabetes and heart disease.
A woman's family was found to be the primary driver of her decisions relating to food procurement, preparation, and consumption. These observations for urban Delhi are consistent with the results of 1 previous study on drivers of food choice in rural Kerala, which found a strong influence of the preferences of husbands and children at each stage of food choice.
Interestingly, this study found that in some cases, strong spousal influences led to unhealthy food decisions including eating outside food, such as pizza. Two overweight women shared that they would prefer to eat less rice or cook with less oil, but that they could not make these dietary changes because of their husbands' preferences. This is a key insight for planning future interventions for obesity prevention in this young adult population. There are several examples of this in the nutrition intervention literature, for example, a weight loss intervention among Mexican American women found that participants in the group who attended classes with their spouses and children on making changes in eating habits and exercise behavior (family group) had the greatest weight loss compared with participants attending individual-based classes with dietitians (individual group) or participants receiving printed materials on nutrition and exercise (control group).
Involving grandmothers (eg, mother or mothers-in-law) also led to improvements in nutritional behaviors. For example, a nutrition education intervention in Senegal found that involving grandmothers positively influenced infant feeding practices of women of reproductive age.
Cultural perceptions and habits established in childhood emerged as substantial, if sometimes subconscious, drivers of decisions relating to both foods and beverages in this sample of young adult women in Delhi. This is consistent with the category of cultural ideals from Furst et al,
which are formed and established within the context of the life course, so that standards for food choice are internalized and imprinted on people's consciousness. These influences inform and shape people's personal systems, including unconsciously functioning strategies that may occur in a food-related choice situation.
Although this may suggest that these habits will be difficult to change in the context of policies aimed at improving dietary intake, results of this study also suggest that marriage may be a critical transition point at which behavior change is possible. Adopting what Verplanken and Wood
called downstream-plus-context-change interventions, educational campaigns and interventions should target men and women during the natural shift in environment and subsequent lifestyle behaviors that occurs when they marry.
Indeed, a previous study conducted among South Asian patients in the UK found that women would be more willing to participate in clinical research if their husbands were also involved,
and pilot studies focused on chronic diseases in Chiapas, Mexico, and on the Texas–Mexico border found that involvement of spouses and other family members in the intervention is highly valued.
The role of convenience, of being able to purchase all items under 1 roof, largely influenced procurement and where families bought food items. This idea is reminiscent of the rise in general merchandise superstores in the US over the past few decades. The gradual replacement of smaller, specialty grocery stores with these superstores reflects the growing demand of American consumers to consolidate their shopping trips.
The data suggest that this ideology of convenient, 1-stop shopping seems to have taken hold with young adult women in Delhi; many reported that they purchase all of their food items from 1 location owing to price and convenience. As more women enter the workforce in India, this convenience factor may become even more influential. However, food is an essential part of Indian culture, and the researchers hypothesize that the priority placed on family members enjoying meals together, which serves the function of socialization and bringing unity to the family, is unlikely to fade.
Food safety and health consistently emerged as deterrents to eating outside food during the in-depth interviews. This concern was reiterated and confirmed by the pile sort activity. The idea of food from Delhi being less pure than food from home villages also arose in several interviews. This may explain why such a high proportion of the Indian population fails to meet the World Health Organization's recommended intake of fruits and vegetables.
Food safety is a rising concern in Delhi, where poor agricultural, manufacturing, and hygiene practices contribute to a high rate of produce contamination with pesticide residues and heavy metals.
These concerns may contribute to inadequate vegetable and fruit consumption in this population, which has been identified as a leading risk factor for NCDs such as diabetes and heart disease.
Food safety is 1 component of the broader concept of food security, defined by the Food and Agriculture Organization as having “access to sufficient, safe and nutritious food that meets [an individual's] dietary needs and food preferences for an active and healthy life.”
However, even in the poorest wealth tertile, inadequate access to sufficient food was not identified as a barrier. In several instances in this socioeconomic group, however, there were examples of women who were unable to consume foods that they preferred, such as fruits, owing to economic constraints. Thus, in addition to concerns about food safety, cost is likely a significant barrier to increasing fruit intake for some population groups.
Cost interacted with most of the primary themes, particularly with respect to convenience and procurement, when women across wealth tertiles stated that they chose a specific market because of the cheap costs. These markets, such as Big Bazaar Outlet and Kendriya Bhandars (coops run by the Government of India, originally set up as a welfare project in 1963),
were chosen for their dependable, high-quality products at lower prices. Some wealth-related distinctions emerged, however. For example, women in the highest wealth tertile expressed that they often did not have time to cook, which resulted in eating out or purchasing premade foods. Moreover, the concept of eating whatever was available in the house (which often meant eating the same food for consecutive meals) was common only among women in the lower and middle wealth tertiles. Interestingly, the importance of cost as a driver of food choice in the pile sort activity did not differ across wealth index tertiles. This contrasts with findings from the Kerala study,
which found cost to be a primary driver, but it may reflect the fact that the Kerala study was conducted in rural areas whereas the current study was conducted in an urban area where individuals may face fewer financial barriers to obtaining food.
Nearly every participant denied the influence of television advertisements or other media on their food choices when asked in both the in-depth interview and the pile sort activity. Women named specific products such as Maggi Noodles (Nestlé) and milk supplements (eg, Bournvita [Cadbury] and Horlicks [GlaxoSmithKline]), most often related to their children's diet, which were advertised on television. Some women also mentioned that they had received pamphlets with newspapers about free home deliveries from restaurants, but they did not admit that these advertisements influenced their food choices. Studies have shown that marketing has a huge influence on food choice, especially as people move toward processed foods, but most people are unaware of their own susceptibility to the media.
In fact, marketing works best when people are not conscious of it, because when people realize they are being marketed to, they can form a negative connection. Therefore, the uniform denial of the study participants to the influence of marketing on their food choices suggests the potential power of media to formulate a decision subconsciously and the need for innovative research to understand exposure to advertisements and the influence of marketing on food choices in this context.
The heightened awareness of health, as indicated by the pile sort activity and numerous narrative statements, may in fact be protecting young adult women in Delhi against the negative effects of the nutrition transition. Health was found to be important across all wealth categories, and indeed knowledge of nutrition, such as too much oil being unhealthy, was found in the lowest and highest wealth tertiles. Nonetheless, the cultural perceptions that homemade food is universally healthy and outside food is universally unhealthy and that junk foods are Western food but do not include processed packaged foods, are pervasive in this population. Similarly, 1 previous study conducted among 20- to 27-year-old college students in Delhi also found a strong preference for homemade food over outside food.
In addition, in this sample, no woman explicitly mentioned high-sugar or high-salt foods as being unhealthy; the focus was always on oil. These represent opportunities to improve nutrition knowledge and the healthfulness of Indian diets.
This study had several strengths. First, it was 1 of the first studies to examine drivers of food choice qualitatively in this setting, 1 in which the nutrition transition is still under way. Second, the use of multiple methods, as well as mixed methods, allowed the researchers to reach saturation and examine patterns, or their lack, across wealth tertiles and BMI categories. Incorporating the quantitative pile sorting activity into the interviews allowed for a triangulation of methods that further verified the qualitative results; aside from convenience, the majority of themes and drivers were universal across the sample. Finally, coding of transcripts was confirmed by multiple investigators individually to ensure data validity and reduce bias.
A limitation of this study was the use of convenience sampling to recruit participants based on geographical proximity, which may limit the generalizability of the results to the entire Delhi area and, more broadly, to urban cities in India. Moreover, although the sample was diverse in terms of wealth index, overall the sample was well educated (43% had at least a college degree) and predominantly Hindu (95% of participants), which further limits generalizability more broadly to urban cities in India. Delhi is a cosmopolitan city with individuals and families from several different states in India, and each state in India has its own food culture, choices, and habits. These results therefore represent a group of women from diverse backgrounds and culture in India. Unfortunately, information was unavailable regarding the specific area from which women who had migrated to Delhi had originated. However, drivers of food choice are likely to be context-specific, hence the importance of conducting mixed-methods research including qualitative interviews. The results are not meant to be generalizable to the entire population, but rather to inform context-specific, targeted interventions. Another limitation relating to sampling was that these women were participating in a larger cohort study, which has the overall aim of cardiometabolic disease surveillance. Thus, there may have been social desirability bias in the responses relating to diet and health. One limitation faced by all qualitative researchers is “the unitization problem,” which is the lack of clear guidelines regarding the appropriate unit of analysis for coding transcripts.
This study used the excerpts from CB's coded transcripts as the unit of analysis for comparison with transcripts coded by VG and DK. However, alternative approaches to unitization are possible and would lead to different percent agreement values.
Nonetheless, clearly defined codes and the ability to reconcile discrepancies with discussion lend strength to the analysis.
Implications for Research and Practice
This mixed-methods study found that young adult women in Delhi rely primarily on the preferences of their families, including their husbands, children, and in-laws, when making choices about food. Whereas women are often considered the gatekeepers to food in the household,
this study indicates that their choices are still largely driven by preferences of their families. Future qualitative studies on drivers of food choice might consider interviewing husbands and mothers-in-law in addition to young women, for a comprehensive family perspective. Results of this study also suggest that marriage may be a critical transition point at which behavior change is possible. To date, no such interventions targeting diet-related NCD prevention among recently married couples have been implemented in India, but this study's findings together with previous research suggest this is a promising line of research.
Such studies would need to take into account issues relating to female empowerment and autonomy. Another important consideration is that many food decisions are habits established in childhood, and therefore they may prove difficult to change via behavioral intervention. Nonetheless, the women who were interviewed put a high value on food, perhaps because most were mothers, and this is promising because they seem open to learning about and improving their diets. Interventions targeting the prevention of obesity, diabetes, and other diet-related diseases in India could focus on recently married couples, involving the extended family in small group counseling sessions, to establish healthy dietary practices grounded in traditional foods during the preconception period to promote health for current and future generations.
Acknowledgments
Funding was provided by the Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health. The authors wish to thank the CARRS participants who volunteered their time for this study. They would also like to thank the CARRS field team for their assistance with scheduling all participant interviews and Dr Roopa Shivashankar, the CARRS project coordinator.
Supplementary Data
The following is the supplementary data to this article:
in: Pequegnat W. Stover E. Boyce C.A. How to Write a Successful Research Grant Application: A Guide for Social and Behavioral Scientists. 2nd ed. Springer,
New York, NY2011: 153-172